Through the Life Sciences Summer Undergraduate Research Program (LSSURP) at the University of Minnesota, Ines investigated bilingual infants’ perceptual narrowing, their ability to identify phonemes native to their own languages, in their first year of life. They compared “typically developing” monolingual and bilingual infants’ phoneme sensitivity and found that the two groups of infants were largely similar — both could identify phonemes of all languages around 4 months of age, but were only able to distinguish phonemes from their native language(s) around 8-12 months of age. Due to the potential confounds of acoustic similarity in stimuli selection and the similarity between chosen languages, more research is necessary to clarify the bilingual phoneme sensitivity timeline.
Katherine Emmons, Annette Estes Stephen Dager, Adrian KC Lee, Bonnie K. Lau
Multitalker speech perception, the ability to listen to one speaker in the presence of several competing speakers, is an important skill used in everyday life. Spatial cues, or timing and intensity differences between the two ears, play a critical role in segregating competing talkers and help listeners localize where the voice of interest is coming from. For example, if the speaker of interest is standing to a listener’s right, their voice will arrive at the right ear faster and louder than the left ear. Normal-hearing (NH) neurotypical (NT) listeners can use spatial cues to selectively attend to a specific speaker, while ignoring competing speakers’ voices. In this study, researchers found that participants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may struggle using spatial cues to selectively attend to one talker in the presence of competing talkers.
Research question:
Can autistic young adults use spatial cues to selectively attend to one of three simultaneous sentences?
What did the researchers do?
Researchers asked 24 participants (12 ASD participants; 12 comparison group particpants) aged 21-23 years, to participate in a study at the University of Washington. Participants completed a multitalker listening task where they listened to three people talking at once. Each person said a sentence with a keyword, a color, and a number. Participants were asked to report back the color and number spoken by the target talker, indicated by the key word, “Charlie.” This speaker was known as the ‘target talker.’ The target talker was always a male voice that came from directly in front of the participant.
Participants were asked to indicate the color and number spoken by the target talker using a response panel.
Researchers measured speech perception thresholds in terms of target-to-masker ratios (TMR; the volume level difference between the target talker and the masker talkers) for each participant.
What were the results?
Participants in the ASD group were able to complete the multitalker listening task, though they required higher target-to-masker ratios (i.e., the target talker had to be louder than the two competing talkers) compared to comparison group participants.
Why is this important?
Results suggest adults with ASD may have difficulty using spatial cues to separate simultaneous auditory streams. Autistic adults may benefit from having their communication partner step away from competing talkers in multitalker situations, so that their communication partners voice becomes louder than other competing voices in the room.